Types of Research Designs
Experiments
Description |
|
Classic Example |
-
Randomly divide sample of allergy sufferers into two groups.
One gets new allergy medicine, the other gets placebo. Then
symptom severity is measured for each person. If no
statistical difference between the two groups, we conclude
the medicine doesn't work
|
Complex Example |
|
Pros |
-
Random assignment means that statistical controls are less
needed. Aside from simulations, experiments give us best
shot at establishing causality.
-
For practical purposes, we basically accept results of
experiments as indicative of causality
|
Cons |
-
Ethical considerations prevent us from using experimental
method in huge number of cases. Can't randomly assign people
to smoking vs non-smoking groups.
-
Often have to be conducted in lab, limiting realism and
therefore generalizability
-
Often a pain to construct
|
Notes |
-
"Treatment" often refers to some kind of active
intervention, such applying some therapy. But doesn't have
to be as the complex example shows.
-
Complex example also shows that the independent variable
does not have to be categorical
-
Data do not have to be explicitly collected at multiple
points in time, but since the treatment/control assignment
is done before measuring outcomes, in effect this is
longitudinal
|
Quasi Experiments
Description |
|
Classic Example |
-
Randomly divide sample of allergy sufferers into two groups.
One gets new allergy medicine, the other gets placebo. Then
symptom severity is measured for each person. If no
statistical difference between the two groups, we conclude
the medicine doesn't work.
|
Complex Example |
|
Pros |
|
Cons |
-
Ethical considerations prevent us from using experimental
method in huge number of cases. Can't randomly assign people
to smoking vs non-smoking groups.
-
Often have to be conducted in lab, limiting realism and
therefore generalizability
-
Often a pain to construct
|
Notes |
-
"Treatment" often refers to some kind of active
intervention, such applying some therapy. But doesn't have
to be as the complex example shows.
-
Complex example also shows that the independent variable
does not have to be categorical
-
Data do not have to be explicitly collected at multiple
points in time, but since the treatment/control assignment
is done before measuring outcomes, in effect this is
longitudinal
|
2. Survey
- good for handling moderate number of variables
- longitudinal surveys good for testing causal arguments
3. Participant Observation
- good for theory construction and holistic assessments
4. Simulations
- excellent for testing logical inferences (i.e., a claim that x&y
can lead to z via a certain process)
5. Archival
- using secondary sources. very convenient
|
|
Send mail to
sborgatti@uky.edu with
questions or comments about this web site. Copyright © 2008
by Steve Borgatti. Last modified:
08/29/09.
Visit Analytic Technologies
for social
network analysis software and
cultural domain analysis software. |